翻訳と辞書
Words near each other
・ Mutual capacitance
・ Mutual coherence
・ Mutual coherence (linear algebra)
・ Mutual coherence (physics)
・ Mutual combat
・ Mutual Construction Co v Komati Dam Joint Venture
・ Mutual Core
・ Mutual credit
・ Mutual Defense Assistance Act
・ Mutual Defense Treaty (United States – Philippines)
・ Mutual Defense Treaty Between the United States and the Republic of Korea
・ Mutual engagement
・ Mutual exchange
・ Mutual exclusion
・ Mutual exclusivity
Mutual exclusivity (psychology)
・ Mutual Feeling
・ Mutual Film
・ Mutual Film Company
・ Mutual Film Corp. v. Industrial Commission of Ohio
・ Mutual Friends
・ Mutual Friends (album)
・ Mutual Friends (film)
・ Mutual fund
・ Mutual Fund Dealers Association
・ Mutual Fund Directors Forum
・ Mutual fund fees and expenses
・ Mutual fund separation theorem
・ Mutual fund trust
・ Mutual funds in India


Dictionary Lists
翻訳と辞書 辞書検索 [ 開発暫定版 ]
スポンサード リンク

Mutual exclusivity (psychology) : ウィキペディア英語版
Mutual exclusivity (psychology)
Mutual exclusivity, in developmental psychology, is the assumption that only one label can be applied to each object in early word learning.〔 Mutual exclusivity is a ''constraint'' that is often discussed by domain-specific accounts of language as limiting infants' hypotheses about the possible meanings of words.〔Clark, E. V. (2009). Lexical meaning. In E. L. Bavin (Ed.), ''The Cambridge Handbook of Child Language'' (pp. 283-300). New York: Cambridge University Press.〕〔Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hollich, G. (2000). An emergentist coalition model for word learning: Mapping words to objects is a product of the interaction of multiple cues. In R. M. Golinkoff, K. Hirsh-Pasek, L. Bloom, L. B. Smith, A. L. Woordward, N. Akhtar,…G. Hollich (Eds.), ''Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition'' (pp. 136-164). New York: Oxford University Press.〕
Mutual exclusivity might influence an infant's word learning in four different ways. First, mutual exclusivity might influence the infant's decision about the reference of a new word. For example, if a child is faced with a familiar object for which they already have a label, and an unfamiliar object, which they have not yet labelled, they are likely to map a novel word onto the unfamiliar object. This is called the ''disambiguation effect''.〔 Next, the bias might cause an infant to change the extension of a familiar word. In this case, a child might remove 'wolf' from their extension of 'dog' upon hearing an animal labelled a 'wolf.' This can be called the ''correction effect''.〔 As well, the bias might compel an infant to reject a new word, for instance, rejecting 'wolf' in favour of 'dog.' This is the ''rejection effect''.〔 Lastly, mutual exclusivity might influence word generalization. In this case, if a child knows an object to have a particular name, the child should not generalize other names to it. This is known as the ''restriction effect''.〔Merriman, W. E., & Bowman, L. L. (1989). The mutual exclusivity bias in children’s word learning. ''Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 54''(3-4, Serial No. 220).〕
There are conflicting positions in the literature regarding the mutual exclusivity bias. Some theorists believe that children possess this bias from the start of word learning, others argue that they never have the bias, and yet others believe that the bias is acquired throughout early childhood.〔
==References==



抄文引用元・出典: フリー百科事典『 ウィキペディア(Wikipedia)
ウィキペディアで「Mutual exclusivity (psychology)」の詳細全文を読む



スポンサード リンク
翻訳と辞書 : 翻訳のためのインターネットリソース

Copyright(C) kotoba.ne.jp 1997-2016. All Rights Reserved.